Southern Pied-babbler
Encyclopedia
The Southern Pied-babbler (Turdoides bicolor) is a species of bird
Bird
Birds are feathered, winged, bipedal, endothermic , egg-laying, vertebrate animals. Around 10,000 living species and 188 families makes them the most speciose class of tetrapod vertebrates. They inhabit ecosystems across the globe, from the Arctic to the Antarctic. Extant birds range in size from...

 in the Timaliidae family.
It is found in Botswana
Botswana
Botswana, officially the Republic of Botswana , is a landlocked country located in Southern Africa. The citizens are referred to as "Batswana" . Formerly the British protectorate of Bechuanaland, Botswana adopted its new name after becoming independent within the Commonwealth on 30 September 1966...

, Namibia
Namibia
Namibia, officially the Republic of Namibia , is a country in southern Africa whose western border is the Atlantic Ocean. It shares land borders with Angola and Zambia to the north, Botswana to the east and South Africa to the south and east. It gained independence from South Africa on 21 March...

, South Africa
South Africa
The Republic of South Africa is a country in southern Africa. Located at the southern tip of Africa, it is divided into nine provinces, with of coastline on the Atlantic and Indian oceans...

, and Zimbabwe
Zimbabwe
Zimbabwe is a landlocked country located in the southern part of the African continent, between the Zambezi and Limpopo rivers. It is bordered by South Africa to the south, Botswana to the southwest, Zambia and a tip of Namibia to the northwest and Mozambique to the east. Zimbabwe has three...

.
Its natural habitat
Habitat
* Habitat , a place where a species lives and grows*Human habitat, a place where humans live, work or play** Space habitat, a space station intended as a permanent settlement...

 is dry savanna
Savanna
A savanna, or savannah, is a grassland ecosystem characterized by the trees being sufficiently small or widely spaced so that the canopy does not close. The open canopy allows sufficient light to reach the ground to support an unbroken herbaceous layer consisting primarily of C4 grasses.Some...

.

Species description

The southern pied babbler is a medium-sized (75-95 g) cooperatively breeding passerine bird. Groups range in size from 2-16 adults, but pairs are rare. The species is sexually monomorphic, with males and females indistinguishable from physical characteristics. Each group comprises a dominant breeding pair that monopolise access to breeding opportunities http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2011/03/17/beheco.arr018.short?rss=1. Recent genetic research has confirmed that these dominant pairs are responsible for more than 95% of young hatched http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2011/03/17/beheco.arr018.short?rss=1 Occasional mixed parentage has been observed, but is predictable in most cases: subordinates primarily gain parentage when a new (unrelated) immigrant disperses into the group, or a new group is founded http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2011/03/17/beheco.arr018.short?rss=1. All group members cooperate to help raise the young hatched from a single clutch. Clutch size varies between 2 and 5, with a modal clutch size of three. Cooperative behaviours include: provisioning young (both in the nest and post-fledging), sentinel behaviour, territory border defense, teaching behaviour and babysitting behaviour (where semi-independent fledglings follow adults between foraging sites and away from predators). The breeding season extends from late-September to early April, although this varies between years and is strongly rain-dependent. Groups can raise up to three successful clutches per breeding season. Average incubation time is 14 days, and average time between hatching and fledging is 16 days. Fledging time varies according to group size
Group size measures
Many animals, including humans, tend to live in groups, herds, flocks, bands, packs, shoals, or colonies of conspecific individuals. The size of these groups, as expressed by the number of participant individuals, is an important aspect of their social environment...

: small groups tend to fledge their young earlier than large groups http://www.zoo.cam.ac.uk/zoostaff/larg/pages/Nikki.html. Post-fledging, young are poorly mobile, unable to fly, and rely entirely on adult group members for food. Fledgling foraging efficiency develops slowly, and fledglings can continue to be provisioned by adults for up to four months post-fledging. The amount of care that young receive during this stage has long-term effects: fledglings that receive care for the longest periods tend to be heavier and better foragers than their counterparts. In addition, they are more likely to successfully disperse from their natal group and consequently begin reproducing earlier than their 'failed-disperser' counterparts http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/18/6/994?rss=1.

Behaviour

Aggression toward fledglings is most commonly observed when the dominant pair have begun to incubate another brood. During this period, begging fledglings will be punished by parents using aggressive behaviour such as jumping on the youngster http://www.zoo.cam.ac.uk/zoostaff/larg/pages/Nikki.html. In all cases, fledglings stop begging immediately following attack. Brood overlap results in a distinctive division of labour, with subordinate adults continuing to care for fledglings while the dominant pair concentrate their effort on the new brood. Owing to the extended period of post-fledging care in this species, this can result in dependent young from multiple broods being raised simultaneously.

Pied babblers are strongly territorial, and defend their borders using wing and vocal displays on a near daily basis. These fights rarely lead to physical aggression and injury from such fights is very rare. Groups defend the same territory year-round and small groups tend to lose portions of their territory to larger neighbouring groups.

Research on pied babblers has provided the first ever evidence of teaching behaviour in an avian species http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6W9W-4R2XCWV-1&_user=635696&_coverDate=01%2F31%2F2008&_rdoc=3&_fmt=summary&_orig=browse&_srch=doc-info(%23toc%236693%232008%23999249998%23677453%23FLA%23display%23Volume)&_cdi=6693&_sort=d&_docanchor=&_ct=36&_acct=C000033878&_version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=635696&md5=e3428a6ec7eb0c8aa0bec91d8a1ac341. Pied babblers teach their young by giving a specific purr call each time they deliver food. Young learn to associate this call with food and reach out of the nest each time they hear it. Adults exploit this association to encourage young to fledge by giving the purr call at a distance from the nest, enticing young to follow them http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6W9W-4PRHM0G-7&_user=635696&_coverDate=11%2F30%2F2007&_rdoc=1&_fmt=summary&_orig=article&_cdi=6693&_sort=v&_docanchor=&view=c&_ct=23&_acct=C000033878&_version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=635696&md5=c9d13f85fe52fedab5fbe49258e20d5e. Post-fledging, adults continue to use the call to encourage young to move between foraging areas or away from predators. This call is also used to recruit independent fledglings to a rich foraging site http://www.current-biology.com/content/article/abstract?uid=PIIS0960982206017805, and may thus provide young with information on where to forage to locate rich food sources.

Research on pied babblers has also provided evidence of task partitioning behaviour http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/content/19/6/1136.abstract?sid=c49fcbf4-fa2d-4042-9dc3-f772489841a7. In this species, the dominant pair are able to leave their dependent young in the care of helpers and initiate a new brood. This allows brood overlap: several broods of dependent young can be raised at the same time. Such a behaviour highlights the benefits of cooperative breeding: many helpers allow breeders to invest in more broods. Parents initiate this task partitioning by aggressively punishing offspring that beg at them for food http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/4/1/23.abstract. This repeated punishment results in young fledglings begging for food from helpers rather than their parents: freeing up their parents to breed again http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/4/1/23.abstract.

Interspecific interactions

Pied babblers have a complex interspecific interaction with the kleptoparasitic fork-tailed drongo, Dicrurus adsimilis. Drongos perch above and follow babbler groups between foraging sites and give alarm calls each time a predator is seen. When drongos are present, babblers invest less time in sentinel behaviour. However, drongos occasionally give false alarm calls and then swoop down to steal the food items that the foraging babblers have dropped upon hearing an alarm call. To avoid the cost of kleptoparasitism
Kleptoparasitism
Kleptoparasitism or cleptoparasitism is a form of feeding in which one animal takes prey or other food from another that has caught, collected, or otherwise prepared the food, including stored food...

, large babbler groups, which have enough group members to participate in sentinel behaviour, do not tolerate drongos and aggressively chase them away from the group. Consequently, they suffer very few losses to kleptoparasitic attack. However, small groups do not have enough group members to provide sentinel behaviour without affecting time invested in other behaviours such as foraging or provisioning young. These groups therefore tolerate occasional kleptoparasitic attacks in return for the sentinel duties that drongos provide http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/18/2/324.

Young pied babblers have difficulty handling larger food items such as scorpions, skinks and solifuges, and take a lot longer to break these food items down than adults http://www.springerlink.com/content/j7pg3765h6161g01/. This makes them ideal victims for attacks by fork-tailed drongos: research has revealed that drongos specifically target young babblers for kleptoparasitic attacks and gain greater foraging success by doing so http://www.springerlink.com/content/j7pg3765h6161g01/

The Pied Babbler Research Project

The Pied Babbler Research Project was established by Dr Amanda Ridley in 2003 for the purpose of studying many aspects of cooperative breeding behaviour over the long-term http://www.fitzpatrick.uct.ac.za/docs/piedbabb.html. The population comprises fully habituated groups of wild pied babblers. The average number of groups in the population varies between 10-18 each year. Research is conducted continuously by scientists and postgraduate students and involves investigations into population dynamics, the causes and consequences of helping behaviour, sexual selection, foraging ecology, interspecific interactions, vocal communication, parent-offspring conflict, kin recognition, maternal effects, physiology and reproductive conflict http://www.fitzpatrick.uct.ac.za/docs/piedbabb.html.

External links

  • Pied Babbler Research Project http://www.www.fitzpatrick.uct.ac.za/docs/piedbabb.html
The source of this article is wikipedia, the free encyclopedia.  The text of this article is licensed under the GFDL.
 
x
OK